The Effects of Enriched and Typical Laboratory Environments on Object Investigation in Old Sprague Dawley Rats
ثبت نشده
چکیده
The purpose of this study was to observe the differences in object investigation between two groups of aged Sprague Dawley rats reared in an enriched and a typical laboratory environment. Research has shown that enrichment has a positive effect on behavior of young and mature rats. Our research question was: Will this positive effect on behavior be retained into old age? Object investigation was measured by recording exploratory activity and overall time spent with objects in an open field. Twelve rats were tested with six in each group. There was no significant difference found in bouts of behavior between the typical and enriched group on Day 1 or Day 2. However, there was a significant difference between the time engaged with objects on Day 1 but not on Day 2. Overall, the results of the study did not support the hypothesis that the enriched group would engage in significantly more object investigation, measured by bouts of behavior and time duration, than the typical laboratory group. Further research on the effects of environment on behavior of the old is important to understand how to maximize functioning through environmental stimulation in old age. Introduction Dating back to the time of Darwin, there has been a fascination with the general curiosity of animals and humans alike (Renner & Seltzer, 1991). Darwin studied this curiosity by “placing a live snake in a bag into the cages in the monkey house and the London Zoological Gardens” (as cited in Renner, 1987). Darwin’s description of the monkey’s reactions was that “they could not resist taking a momentary peak” (as cited in Renner, 1987). Over time, psychologists have further investigated curiosity and have called it animal exploration or investigation. Thinus-Blanc et al. (1987) investigated exploratory behavior in hamsters placed in an open field by manipulating the distances between objects and topological relationships. The results indicated a renewal of exploration after the experimenters affected the spatial relations of the objects but not after they affected the distance. Similarly, a study done by Dubois et al. (1999) measured behavioral bouts of Wedgecapped Capuchin monkeys directed toward objects to address whether location affects activity. Overall, results showed a great deal of betweenand within-subject variability. Renner and Seltzer (1991) defined several exploratory and investigative behaviors in rats in terms of their molar characteristics (e.g., large units of behavior) and studied how these change as a result of repeated opportunities to explore the same environment. The results indicated the activity levels remained the same over the period of observation, and the amount of time spent interacting with objects increased initially followed by a decrease. In an additional study, Renner and Seltzer (1994) suggested that behavioral grammars can be used to predict individual animals’ References Fagot, J., & Vauclair, J. (1991). Manual laterality in nonhuman primates: A distinction between handedness and manual specialization. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 76-89. Hopkins, W. D., Stoinski, T. S., Kukas, K. E., Ross, S. R., & Wesley, M.J. (2003). Comparative assessment of handedness for a coordinated bimanual task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 302-308. MacNeilage, P. F., Studdert-Kennedy, M.G., & Lindblom, B. (1987). Primate handedness reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 247-303. Milliken, G. W., Forsythe, C. & Ward, J. P. (1989). Multiple measures of hand-use lateralization in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 262-268. Milliken, G. W., Stafford, D. K., Dodson, D. L., Pinger, C. D. & Ward, J. P. (1991). Analyses of feeding lateralization in the small-eared bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii): A comparison with the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 274-285. Papademetriou, E., Sheu, C., & Michel, G.F. (2005). A meta-analysis of primate hand preferences, particularly for reaching. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 33-48. Ward, J. P., Milliken, G. W., Dodson, D. L., Stafford, D. K. & Wallace, M. (1990). Handedness as a function of sex and age in a large population of lemurs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 167-173. Westergaard, G.C., Kuhn, H.E., & Suomi, S.J. (1998). Bipedal posture and hand preference in humans and other primates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 56-63. Page 36 Oshkosh Scholar Page 37 Oshkosh Scholar Method Participants The subjects used in this study were 12 female Sprague-Dawley rats (Rattus norvegicus). Of these 12 rats, 6 served as the control group raised in a typical laboratory environment, and 6 served as an experimental group raised in an enriched environment. During the course of this experiment, all subjects had access to food and water ad lib, except during the brief observation periods. Lights were on from 0700 to 1900 hr daily, until two weeks prior to testing when the light-dark cycle was changed to 0500 to 1700 hr daily for testing purposes. Apparatus Observation Area The observation area, known as an open field, was a 111.76 cm circle surrounded by 43.18 cm high wooden walls. Subjects were transported to the observation area individually in a Plexiglas® cage and placed in the center of the circular area as determined by the pre-measured diameter. The arena was illuminated by a red light, which is virtually undetectable to rats, allowing the researchers to view the rats’ activities. Stimulus Objects Objects were classified as either manipulable or nonmanipulable depending upon the rat’s ability to move each object. Objects consisted of random household items such as a spoon, a sock, and a textbook. A total of four different objects, two manipulable and two nonmanipulable, were present in the open field area during each testing block. Each of the eight objects was replaced by a similar object for Day 2 of testing. Videotaping Equipment Behaviors were videotaped on a Sony 990-Handycam Camera, serial number 308928901, which was placed directly above the area of observation. Procedure The rats used in this study were subjects of a prior study. Although there was a clear difference between the enriched rats and the typical rats as seen in their interactions with humans, the enrichment condition would have been constant if there had been control of their environments since weaning for this study. At 32 days the enriched rats (n = 6) were placed in a Plexiglas® cage measuring 70 cm x 70 cm x 46 cm. It was filled with wooden toys, nibble bars, a running wheel, and golf balls. The typical laboratory rats (n = 6), also at age 32 days, were housed in pairs in 28 cm x 21 cm x 19 cm empty metal cages. All rats were tested four times in a six-unit T-maze during the prior study (Rauscher, 2005). At age 62 days the enriched rats were housed in the same conditions as the typical laboratory rats until the conclusion of the study. At this time the enriched rats were returned to their previous enriched condition. All rats were held daily during the previous study to accustom them to human contact, which ended at age 122 days. It is important to note for this study that there was a difference in the ages of the enriched and typical laboratory rats. The typical rats were born approximately interactions with stimulus objects. They defined behavioral grammars as the bouts of specific object interaction observed in different rats. Overall, they found there were no stereotypical object-investigation behaviors. Rather, they observed many individual differences in the rats’ behaviors. Many neuroscience studies have used exploration as a dependent variable for their research in order to look at brain functions (Patra, Mohanty, & Das, 1984; Ricceri, Calamandrei, & Berger Sweeney, 1997; Young, Wintink, & Kalynchuk, 2004). Todorovic et al. (2003) examined both behavioral and immunological functions to find a possible link between the two during the aging process. The study found a significant correlation between age-related reduction in exploratory activity and reduced capability of the immune system, suggesting such a link exists. Increased object exploration has been studied in relation to enriched and typical laboratory environments. Enrichment generally consists of housing animals together in a complex and stimulating environment, which has been shown to enhance interactions with littermates and objects. “The behavioral activity of interactions with objects in the enriched condition has been shown to lead to a relatively enduring change in behavior, that of alteration in exploratory behavior” (Renner & Rosenzweig, 1987, p. 89). Renner (1987) studied the plasticity of exploratory behaviors in adult male rats raised either in a typical laboratory environment or an enriched environment and found that rats from the enriched environment participated in a greater diversity of behaviors related to objects, as well as longer interactions with the objects, than the typical laboratory group. Also, the enriched subjects climbed more than the typical laboratory group on the objects that were nonmanipulable. Enrichment studies have focused on the effects of enrichment on the developing brain and behavior in the young or mature rat but not in the old rat. A computerized literature search located 30 articles using a keyword search of “rats” “exploration” “enriched” in the following data bases: PsycINFO (Psychological Abstracts), (1887 to present); EBSCOhost, (1985 to present); and Wilson Web, (1983 to present). However, when using the keywords “old rats” or “aged rats” along with “exploration” “enriched,” no articles were located, which indicates a gap in the literature knowledge base about how enrichment in young life affects behavior in the old rat. In this study, one aspect of exploratory behavior was investigated—object investigation. The purpose of this study was to observe the differences in object investigation between two groups of aged Sprague Dawley rats reared in either an enriched or typical laboratory environment. Research has shown that enrichment has a positive effect on behavior of young and mature rats. Our research question was: Will this positive effect on behavior be retained into old age? In order to examine the positive effects of an enriched environment on aged rats, object investigatory behavior of an enriched group and a typical laboratory group was recorded and compared. These observations were conducted in an open field where each subject was allowed free rein of the field which consisted of manipulable and nonmanipulable objects. It was hypothesized that the enriched group would engage in significantly more object investigation, measured by bouts and time duration, than the typical laboratory group. Page 38 Oshkosh Scholar Page 39 Oshkosh Scholar significant difference in time duration between the groups on Day 1 which disappearedon Day 2, it is plausible that the plasticity of the typical laboratory old rat’s brain canaccount for the lack of differences in behavior bouts and time duration on Day 2. Thisexplanation would suggest that although old rats were subjected to a typical laboratoryenvironment for the duration of their lives, there remained sufficient brain plasticityfor them to show as high a level of exploratory behavior as the enriched rats on Day2. The results could also indicate that the dependent variables were not sensitive to thetreatment and that other dependent measures should have been chosen, which couldhave revealed findings more in agreement with literature on the positive effects ofenriched atmospheres. Future research is needed to test these possible explanations.There are several limitations of this study and suggestions for further research.As mentioned in the procedure, the rats used in this study were subjects of a priorstudy. Although there was a clear difference between the enriched rats and the typicallaboratory rats as seen in their interactions with humans, it would be advisable to haveconstant control over the environments of both groups of rats from weaning. Also,there were only six rats in each treatment condition. To further generalize the results,a larger n should be used. Furthermore, a within-groups calculation was not conductedin this study, which limited the analysis. This information may have provided furtherexplanation for the results found in the present study and would allow for individualdifferences to be analyzed in future studies. A specific suggestion of additional researchon enrichment would be to raise a group of rats all in a typical laboratory environmentuntil old age and then divide the group equally between the typical laboratory andan enriched environment to see if enrichment in later life only has an effect onobject investigation.Past studies focused primarily on enrichment environments for young ormature rats and failed to compare old rats raised in enriched and typical laboratoryenvironments with respect to object investigation. This study has contributed to thescience of psychology by expanding the body of research on enrichment to find outif positive effects on behavior are retained into old age. Additional studies on thetopic of aging in relation to environment are needed to determine the most adaptiveenvironment for older individuals. Research on the effects of environment on behavioris important to understanding how to maximize functioning through environmentalstimulation in old age. The results could indicate how a stimulating nursing homeenvironment can impact behavior. Overall, this type of research has increasingimportance as age demographics change and the baby boomer cohort moves intoolder adulthood. ReferencesDubois, M., Sampaio, E., Gerard, J. F., Quenette, P.Y., & Muniz, J. (2000). Location-specific re-sponsiveness to environmental perturbations in wedge-capped capuchins (cebus olivacues).International Journal of Primatology, 21(1), 85-101. Patra, S.K., Mohanty, M., & Das, N. (1984). Exploration and activity in the amygdale-ablatedalbino rats. Perspectives in Psychological Research, 7(1). 1-7. Rauscher, F.H. (2005). Longitudinal effects of early vs. late enrichment in rats. Paper submittedto the annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, CA.September 13, 2004, and the enriched rats were born approximately January 7, 2005.Despite the age difference, both groups were considered to be old rats, as supportedby rat study literature. Throughout the present study, the typical laboratory ratswere housed in empty metal cages. The enriched rats were housed in the communalPlexiglas® cage and were exposed to spontaneous bouts of interactions with humanslasting no more than 20 minutes at any one time.Rats in both groups were held for 2-minute intervals for 8 days. Three daysbefore testing, all rats were acclimated to the open area three times for 5 minutes eachand held for 2-minute intervals each. Each rat was tested on two separate consecutivedays for 10-minute blocks each day between the hours of 1800 and 2100. The 10-minute block began when we left the observation room. After 10 minutes, we reenteredthe room and the recording stopped. The testing area was cleaned as necessary toremove waste, but no solvents were used during the 2-day testing period. Operationaldefinitions used to code behaviors are listed in Table 1.During testing, we were blind to whether we were observing the enriched ortypical laboratory rats to eliminate the possibility of experimenter bias. Additionally,coding of the recorded tapes did not begin until a 90% interrater reliability wasestablished using practice rats. ResultsThe total bouts of behavior were tallied for each rat in the typical laboratoryand enriched condition. Bouts on Day 1 were calculated independent of Day 2.The means of the typical laboratory and enriched groups on Day 1 were analyzedwith a one-way analysis of variance; there was no significant difference: F(1, 11) =0.06, p > .05, as seen in Table 2 (see the Appendix for an explanation of statisticalabbreviations and symbols). Similarly, as shown in Table 3, the means on Day 2 werenot significantly different: F(1, 11) = 0.52, p > .05. The total time engaged with objectswas also recorded. The means of the typical laboratory and enriched groups on Day 1were analyzed with a one-way analysis of variance, and these means were found to besignificantly different: F(1, 11) = 5.96, p < .05, as seen in Table 4. However, as seen inTable 5, a difference was not observed on Day 2: F(1, 11) = 0.52, p > .05. DiscussionDespite previous research findings indicating that enrichment had a positiveeffect on behavior of young and mature rats, the results suggested that the behaviorsobserved in the typical laboratory old rats and in the enriched old rats did notsignificantly differ. Although there was a significant difference between the groups intime spent interacting with the objects on Day 1, that difference disappeared on Day 2.Overall, the results of the study did not support the hypothesis that the enriched groupwould engage in significantly more object investigation, measured by bouts of behaviorand time duration, than the typical laboratory group.Our unique research question was: Will this positive effect on behavior beretained into old age? Given the results of this study, two plausible explanations exist.It is plausible that any gains in behavior as a result of an enriched environment arelost in old age. This would suggest that cognitive slowing occurs regardless of whatenvironment a rat is in for the duration of its life. Alternatively, given that there was a Page 40Oshkosh ScholarPage 41Oshkosh Scholar Table 2Analysis of Variance Summary Table _____________________________________________________________________Day 1 Bouts Source of variance SSdfMSFp A6.7516.75.06 p > .05S/A1060.1710 106.02 Total1066.91711_____________________________________________________________________ Note. Was not significant at p = .05 alpha level. A = Enriched v. typical groups; S/A = Variability
منابع مشابه
The Effects of Soy Milk on Histomorphometric Changes of Olfactory Bulb in Neonatal Ovariectomized RatsSprague- Dawley strain
Background & Objective: Soy milk contains isoflavones that comprises the phytoestrogens families. They have structural similarities with mammalian estrogen. This study was done to investigate the effects of soy milk on histomorphometric changes of olfactory bulb in neonatal ovariectomized rats. Materials & Methods: Thirty female rats Sprague- Dawley strain (one-day old) were kept in a standa...
متن کاملEnvironmental enrichment and isolation rearing in the rat: effects on locomotor behavior and startle response plasticity.
BACKGROUND Laboratory rats exhibit behavioral changes that reflect a continuum of early life experience, from isolation-reared to socially reared to enrichment-reared conditions. In this study, we further characterize the behavioral effects of isolation, social, and enriched rearing on locomotor activity, patterns of movement and exploration, startle reactivity, prepulse inhibition (PPI), and h...
متن کاملThe Effects of Aerobic Exercise in Sprague Dawley pregnant Rats on vascular BCL-2,BAX AND eNOS Gene Expression in adult male offspring
Background: One of the most growing diseases is the onset of atherosclerosis which can be started from a fetal age. As a result, the intrauterine environment plays a role at risk of spreading this disease. Changes in vascular function considered to be as an indicator for vascular disease. Effective factors on vascular function are enzyme eNOS and apoptotic regulator factors included BCL-2 an...
متن کاملProtective Effects of Hydro-alcoholic Extract of Apium Graveolens on Carbon Tetrachloride Induced Hepatotoxicity in Sprague-Dawley male Rats
Background & Objective: Apium graveolens has antioxidant property because of its flavonoid compounds. In the present study, we investigated the hepatoprotective effects of hydroalcoholic extract of Apium graveolens in Sprague Dawley male rats treated with carbon tetrachloride (CCl4). Materials & Methods: In this study, 48 rats (weighted 180-200 gram) were divided into six groups each having e...
متن کاملHematologic status in DMBA-induced mammary gland carcinoma in sprague dawley rats
in this study,14 female sprague-dawley rats 55 days old were randomly divided into two control and experimental groups.2ml of 7,12-dimethylbenz{a} anthracene (DMBA) solution(20 mg of DMBA dissolved in 2 ml of corn oil) and 2 ml corn oil was eaten to each animal of test and control group respectively.tumor development was examined by milk line palpation once every week.during 3-5 months followin...
متن کامل